Author
|
Topic: for discussion: DLCs Good or Bad???
|
ebvan Member
|
posted 06-22-2006 06:56 AM
I have always had a bit of a problem with the concept of telling an examinee that we were working together to find the truth and then telling them to lie on the exam with a Directed Lie Comparison Question. 1.What are the different points of view on this subject? 2.Are there certain situations or formats where this works better than others? 3. What does available research say? ebv
IP: Logged |
rnelson Member
|
posted 06-22-2006 08:51 AM
DLC is an interesting argument.There was quite a series of exchanges in the journal a couple of years ago - between honts and Abrams, I believe (now I'll have to check). Abrams made some court testimony and publication that was, in my view, premature and based upon anecdotal experience and opinion that he handed off as research. Sorry to be so critical about this, I really like most of Abrams work and contribution to this field. TES is a DLC test, but its structure and strict operational definition do not generalize well to situations outside its intended use. The TES is one of the best tests yet developed. You can find some info on these (unfortunately) on the anti site. The Utah tests were DLC (or PLC) tests. You can look for Raskin's publications. Plus I think there is some info on these in Matte's book. I am of the opinion that widespread use of the DLC might cause serious changes to the PLC test's useability. The DLC requires clearing telegraphing the control question. It also requires examiners re-think their over-reliance on FFF theoretical foundations, as the FFF response to DLC doesn't hold up. Other psychological and physiological explainations do a better job explaining why the DLC works, and can simultaneously explain PLC reactions. In science, on principle is parsimony - the ability to explain the widest range of phenomena with the simplest possible explaination. FFF cannot explain DLC, and does not therefore meet the requirement for parsimony. Another complication with DLC is that telegraphing the control is an invitation to countermeasure. I believe that use of a peripheral activity sensor (not just motion or movement - any activity in the perepheral nervous system) is critical with the DLC. The peripheral activity sensor tells us through data (not opinion) whether the data from the traditional component sensors is authentic autonomic nervous system data, or autonomic data that is compromised by peripheral nervous system activity. I am aware of one examiner who had an very unfortunate false negative on a DLC test - conducted using ineffective DLCs (directed lie to neutral questions) and no peripheral activity sensor. Effective DLCs still require complexity and personal/ethical compromise. I am not at all convinced the false negative was due to the DLC. r ------------------ "Gentlemen, you can't fight in here, this is the war room." --(Dr. Strangelove, 1964) IP: Logged |
J.B. McCloughan Administrator
|
posted 06-22-2006 08:55 AM
I do not conduct the DLC. Here is a start on research from http://www.nationalpolygraphconsultants.com/polyresults.asp : National Polygraph Consultants, LLC
To print this page, click 'file' and then 'print'. Information on ordering these items follows the list of titles. Articles selected from search: Title: A Critical Analysis of Matte's Analysis of the Directed Lie, FirstAuthor: Charles R. Honts, SecondAuthor: Anne Gordon, Date: 1998, Volume: 27, Number: 4, PageNumber: 241-252, Keywords: directed lie/DLC/criticism, Title: A critical analysis of Matte's analysis of the directed lie, FirstAuthor: Honts, Charles, SecondAuthor: Gordon, Anne, Date: 1998, Volume: 27, Number: 4, PageNumber: 241-252, Keywords: directed lie techniques, validity, Title: An analysis of the psychodynamics of the directed lie control question in the control question technique, FirstAuthor: Matte, James A., Date: 1998, Volume: 27, Number: 1, PageNumber: 56-67, Keywords: comparison question test, CQT, directed lie, PDD, validity, Title: An Analysis of the Psychodynamics' of the Directed Lie Control Question in the Control Question Technique, FirstAuthor: James Allan Mattee, Ph.D., Date: 1998, Volume: 27, Number: 1, PageNumber: 56-67, Keywords: DLC/criticism, Title: The Directed Lie Control Question, FirstAuthor: Stan Abrams Ph.D., Date: 1990, Volume: 19, Number: 4, PageNumber: 26-31, Keywords: techniques-DLCQ/ control questions, Title: The hybrid directed-lie test, the overemphasized comparison question, chimeras and other inventions: A rejoinder to Abrams (1999), FirstAuthor: Honts, Charles R., SecondAuthor: Raskin, David C., ThirdAuthor: Amato, Susan L., FourthAuthor: Gordon, Anne, FifthAuthor: Devitt, Mary, Date: 2000, Volume: 29, Number: 2, PageNumber: 156-168, Keywords: directed lie techniques, validity, Title: The Hybrid Directed-Lie Test, The Overemphasized Comparison Question, Chimeras and Other Inventions: A Rejoinder to Abrams (1999), FirstAuthor: Charles R. Honts, SecondAuthor: David C. Raskin, ThirdAuthor: Susan L. Amato, FourthAuthor: Anne Gordon, FifthAuthor: Mary Devitt, Date: 2000, Volume: 29, Number: 2, PageNumber: 156-168, Keywords: directed lie/DLC/criticism, Title: Validation of potential response elements in the directed lie control question, FirstAuthor: Matte, James A., SecondAuthor: Reuss, Ronald M., Date: 1999, Volume: 28, Number: 2, PageNumber: 124-142, Keywords: directed lie techniques, DLCQ, control question tests, Title: Validation of Potential Response Elements in the Directed-Lie Control Question, FirstAuthor: James A. Matte, SecondAuthor: Ronald M. Reuss, Date: 1999, Volume: 28, Number: 2, PageNumber: 124-142, Keywords: directed lie/DLC/criticism, -------------------------------------------------------------------------------- To order articles from the journal Polygraph, contact the National Office of the American Polygraph Association, PO Box 8037, Chattanooga, TN 37414-0037. You can also contact the National Office by e-mail at apabennett@aol.com, or toll free at 1-800-APA-8037. These citations are provided as a courtesy of the National Polygraph Consultants, which is not affiliated with the APA. IP: Logged |
Barry C Member
|
posted 06-22-2006 10:44 AM
I've used DLCs. They require a proper set up, and you don't just have a person lie to a particular set of questions. There is less research on them, but all of it says the DLC is almost as effective as a PLC test. (The PLC is slightly better, but why give up any accuracy if you don't have to do that?)The info in Matte's book is wrong on the Utah tests (according to Hontss and Raskin's writings and personal communications I've had with them). You can use DLCQs on any Utah test. If you want to know how to run one, read Hont's and Raskin's chapter in Kleiner. I have it in PDF somewhere if anybody wants it. What I don't like about them is they don't "feel" right. How's that for not the least bit scientific? In other words, you can't know a CQ is set until the guy passes the test, unlike the verbal and non-verbal cues you get when you use a well-developed PLCQ. As Ray pointed out, the TES has the best numbers when it comes to a screening exam, and it uses DLCQs. One of the examiners here uses DLC tests exclusivley for IA tests, but I don't recall who it is. IP: Logged |
polypro Member
|
posted 06-22-2006 04:12 PM
There are those who advocate the use of DLC's and then there are those who do not. I can certainly see both ends of the spectrum; however, as I see it you have PLC's, DLC's and the R&I. We all know the inherent problems with R&I, so I want elaborate. Now, what's left? Obviously, I would choose the PLC's over the DLC's in a heartbeat with a couple of exceptions: If an examinee will not commit to PLC's ( confessing to everything thrown at them), the examinee has done extensive research on polygraph and setting comparisons would be problematic, or if I've tested the subject and I must test the subject a second time within a very short period of time, then yes, I would defintely consider DLC's.The only setting in which I might be very hesitant to use DLC's would be the crim enviroment. Why? Well, we all must agree that overcoming the relevant issue can be rather difficult, ever for an innocent examinee, so how does a traffic violation match-up to committing any number of horrible crimes. That leave us with the screening enviroment, which is where the majority of examiners utilize the DLC. In my opinion, if one must or feels the need to use DLC's, this is certainly the setting. These are just thoughts. I'm not, unfortunitely, very well read in research, so I have nothing to back my beliefs. What do you all think? IP: Logged |
CHSBOY Member
|
posted 06-22-2006 04:45 PM
Interesting discussion. Remember that the use of DLCs was originally recommended for non-suspect type screening examinations. The “weak controls,” as they were called in the 1960s, were thought to provide some means of objectively gauging an examinee’s responses during testing which was to that time globally evaluated (by the folks who came up with this technique). They felt that PLCs were too strong for source and asset (and non-suspect) testing, posed problems when testing the same sources often and repeatedly, and occasionally created an unnecessarily adversarial environment. The examinee was prepared during pretest to recognize that polygraph worked, that as long as one was truthful, there would be no (consistent, significant) response during testing. When properly vented during pretest question discussion and review, an examinee was expected to display little to no consistent physiological response to the RQs during testing. The pretest was a little longer and more detailed than what most examiners experience today primarily because of their reliance on and presentation to the examinee of ‘cognitive dissonance’ as the reason for responsiveness during testing vice the FFF thought by so many today to be the reason polygraph works. In this early DLC environment, the examiners were observing the RQs much more than the CQs and as long as there appeared to be ‘normal’ responsiveness at the CQs (and no exaggeration indicative of deliberate manipulations), they were confident that the process was working. When RQs were consistent, timely, and significantly more responsive than CQs, the posttest began. When the opposite was true, the examinee was deemed to be NDI. This is a simple explanation of the technique and one that has not been well understood by many who implemented testing and use following Lou Fuse’s paper in 1982. Most of the polygraph world was used to using PLCs so when they began to experiment with and employ DLCs, the assumption was that they needed to be important, significant, or emotion evoking. All of which is untrue. The examinee needs to be aware that you’re paying attention to his/her answers and they must pay attention to avoid mis-answering. They should also be asked to have something specific to which they are lying. They should not be asked to play mental gymnastics in their mind with the question for a period of time before answering. These concepts are misguided and dangerous in the hands of an inexperienced examiner who might want to accept all resulting tracings as legitimate physiological response. Research has demonstrated that ‘personal involvement’ in the DLC in question provides better results than DLCs which have no personal involvement (“Did you ever violate a traffic law?” vs having a person lie to “Are the lights on?”). Interestingly, the folks who devised this technique did just that but they didn’t have the benefit of research. The argument over which is better, DLCs vs. PLCs, may go on for some time but I think it’s clear both work well and perhaps even have their own arenas where one may provide some benefit over the other (screening vs. specific issue/criminal testing). As Barry noted, some of the recent research by Kircher, et al, indicated a slight edge to the PLC but that was in specific issue testing (I believe). In other projects, particularly several of the screening projects, the DLC modules outperformed the PLC modules (note the TES studies mentioned above and the studies cited within those reports). One last comment for tonight (I’m getting tired), interestingly, while I was taught that DLCs should not be used in issue testing (for ‘balance’, etc., in situations where there was already some emotion evoking issue and perhaps interrogation…PLCs were preferred where I grew up), the studies done by Honts, Kircher, and others usually involve specific issue situations, yet they usually reported highly accurate results using DLCs. Barry mentioned, “they just don’t feel right.” For most who do specific issue testing of folks who are suspected, accused, have been interrogated, the thought of using a CQ which is not “set” is difficult to swallow. And in my opinion, in that situation, a DLC ought not to be used. I don’t think DLCs are the panacea for all polygraph testing but I do think they bring a valuable tool to non-suspect screening which, when conservatively evaluated, lends itself to highly accurate results and the ability to posttest, develop information, and resolve the examination with a high degree of confidence. Now, I’ve done it. I mentioned conservative evaluation. DLCs were never intended (by the original designers) to be evaluated in the same manner as PLCs. Their view of pneumos was critical and primarily to gauge Cm attempts. Research by Horowitz et al. and again more recently by Kircher et al. support this view. Both reported that different rules are necessary for evaluating DLCs. Perhaps a lack of understanding of this need for conservative evaluation can be blamed for the perceived problems with FPs when using DLCs. Recall Don Krapohl’s ‘successive hurdles’ approach and use DLCs as a very conservative measure for initial series screening. This will (in my opinion) lend itself to a politically acceptable, yet very sensitive to deception screening tool for the initial series. Problems leading to interrogation and information development can always be resolved later by break-out testing using PLCs to balance the test for those who have problems passing, have provided information, and are ready for further confirmatory testing to resolve the problematic issue(s). This is just one guy’s opinion most of which was included in a paper in the Journal a few years back to set the record straight and give credit to the guys who devised this technique in the 1960s. Pardon the typos…the old eyes and fingers are failing. Boy what a rambling piece. I’ll keep quiet for a while. Regards, Paul Menges
IP: Logged |
ebvan Member
|
posted 06-23-2006 07:40 AM
OK, I have never used DLC, and have no real plans to begin. For now, the reason that I take that position is mostly caused by a lack of understanding of the concept. I agree they don't "FEEL RIGHT" I don't quite understand yet how a DLC, where the examiner and subject both agree it's OK to lie (ie no consequences), can effectively approach the reaction potential of a contentious RQ where the consequences of being caught telling a lie can be enormous.I would be very interested for someone who actually uses DLC to provide a brief script for introduction of a DLC, to help me get a grasp on using this technique. My mind is still open, but there may not be much room left for new stuff so hurry IP: Logged |
Barry C Member
|
posted 06-23-2006 10:52 AM
They do work. Let's not fail to look at the research out there. You don't simply tell the person to lie to a question. There is a process involved, and you've got to get the person's psych set on the CQs just like in a PLCQ test.You start with a numbers test (known), and you tell the person you know what it looks like when he lies, and that he responds well when he does. You tell him you will use that reaction to compare with the RQs and the CQs on the test (not for greater size even though that is what you'll be doing). You explain that the two question types should look different because he's lying to the DLCQs and - so he says - truthful to the RQs. If the question types look different, and the DLCQs look like the numbers test lie (because you need to know he's maintaining an ability to respond...), then you'll know he's truthful. If, however, both question types look the same, then you'll know they're all lie responses. That creates a fear in the truthful, as they are now motivated to (and fearful of not) respond(ing) well to the DLCQs as they have to in order to convince you of their truthfulness. The liar on the other hand, doesn't want to cooperate (by following your instructions on how to deal with the DLCQs, which I won't discuss now). He is still more concerned about the RQs. I hope that makes some sense. In short, the truthful fear the DLCQs because if they don't react to them, they won't pass the test, something they are highly motivated to do. (I say "fear," but fear really isn't necessary in a polygraph test, but that's a different topic we've discussed already.) Scoring is still the same for a PLCQ and a DLCQ. Use the Utah rules. Breathing is different, but they haven't yet validated a different set of rules for their evaluation. You will get some crazy pneumos on a DLCQ. If you looked at them as you would on a PLCQ, you'd suspect CMs, and that is one of the drawbacks. As I mentioned, I have the chapter in Kleiner's Handbook that explains how to do them and the research behind them. IP: Logged |
ebvan Member
|
posted 06-23-2006 11:10 AM
OK the bulb is beginning to come on, but I'm still very interested in looking at others input on this subject.ebv IP: Logged |
rnelson Member
|
posted 06-23-2006 01:57 PM
Paul Menges has a very interesting article on this in a 2004 polygraph journal.Well said. r ------------------ "Gentlemen, you can't fight in here, this is the war room." --(Dr. Strangelove, 1964) IP: Logged |
Bill2E Member
|
posted 06-24-2006 06:43 AM
DLC's must work, number stim tests work and that is a DL also.IP: Logged |
CHSBOY Member
|
posted 06-24-2006 09:41 AM
Barry's explanation above is on target with what has been advocated when using DLCs since the broader community experimented with and started using DLCs following Lou Fuse's introduction of them in 1981 (or was it '82?). Memory fails me and I'm too lazy to look right now. This method has worked in the field and in the lab. However, for historical reasons, I still have to point out there are some subtle differences between what is done by many today (because it's now been 'researched' using the method of treating DLCs like PLCs; having examinees 'fear' them; or examiners believing they must make the DLCs important and even emotional) and what was done by those who used them initially. Their method was not well understood when introduced by Fuse, primarily because there was little to no exposure to the concept (as they used it) and when the researchers began testing the idea, they truly did not comprehend the sophistication used by Fuse and his mentors (Hitchcok & Dubiel). The early DLC method was to convince an examine (remember: a non-suspect subject looking for job/clearance/or continued access andnot someone who had been interrogated re: a specific crime) that he/she only had to be truthful and cooperative to pass the test. Part of the conditioning took place with the ACQT/Stim test, which in their day, was an unknown number test done in a manner which made it very difficult to miss (see Barland, 1981 & Fuse, 1982). The functions of the DLCs were related to the response activity seen on the ACQT when explaining the questions, to insure the subject recognized the importance of the DLC. Comments such as, "you need to pay attention to avoid misanswering during testing..." when introducing the DLCs, were sufficient to emphasize the importance without requiring the examiner to push the issue of comparison or the need to generate fear in the subject about not passing the test. In fact the entire pretest was done to convince the innocent person that as long as he/she was truthful, they would pass the test. Hence the DLC introduction made clear to a properly prepared subject that they had nothing to fear because they knew the examiner would be able to see where they were lying during testing (and in their case, it would be to the DLCs). Their methods were admittedly more conservative because they were using it in screening and the technique was extremely unintrusive. They had no problem questioning someone respoding to a relevant question and retesting to confirm information developed. Therefore, if there was any significant response to RQs, they questioned. Later on, a numerical analysis was added, I was told, "to help their new examiners learn to evaluate DLCs." While conditioning is important in any pretest, it may be even moreso in the DLC pretest, as originally designed. In this test, there is no emotion-evoking PLC on which to rely to balance things if other parts of the pretest are week. Somehow this philosophy and concept never was emphasized when the DLCs began their entry into the broader community and it is one that is not easily embraced. Especially by examiners who have been used to using PLCs which require the laying of foundations, setting, and some emotion evoking value. The idea is a little different. Actually some of the subtle differences lie at the bottom of the discussion between Honts and Abrams several years back in the APA's Journals and Newsletters. One can get into trouble if DLCs are "pushed" and you don't recognize the distortions which are frequently generated by some of the instructions provided to an examinee during DLC introduction. I pointed this out in the 2004 paper because of some of the concerns voiced across the community about FNs and in some quarters, FPs, when using DLCs. This is more for historial concern rather than trying to point out there is only one way to do it. I've seen DLCs used in a variety of ways, I've noted the research explanations, and have talked with the researchers and those who originally used the "weak controls," as they called them. Some of these 'old guys' complained to me that they felt too many examiners use DLCs today because they are so strong and are less difficult to use (perhaps mistaken thinking). Given how they used the DLCs, I understand their thinking (but am less apt to lean to this judgement since I am aware of the methods espoused in the literature since Fuse's paper). That's how today's examiners have been taught. Their method worked for them. It was their usage of DLCs noted in Barland's 1981 study (hope I'm right on that one!) and the 1989 Barland, Honts, Barger study that awakened the interest of researchers. What has become standard practice in some quarters is not what was practiced by the original designers. And no, I'm not advocating a return to use of the unknown numbers test (and yes, I know that test had some value...). Hopefully, this isn't too disjointed. I'm just trying to add some background and clarity, not preach about the only way to do things. This is a technique that was never really understood as originally used. For whatever that's worth. Again, I'll apologize for typos and rambling but thank you for the forum! PMM IP: Logged |
polypro Member
|
posted 06-24-2006 03:06 PM
So with so much interest in the internet and applicants/examinee's conducting research on PLC's, do you see DLC's to be a thing of the future? Do you see newer and better formats? Will there be combination of the foregoing with the addition of a 4th component? Or, will we stay our present course?IP: Logged |
Barry C Member
|
posted 06-24-2006 05:44 PM
Stan Abrams tried mixing the two, and the debate referred to above took off like a rocket. Right now, it's considered wrong to mix DLCQs and PLCQs, but if I remember correctly, one of the Utah guys was going to research that some more as there really isn't any reason why they shouldn't work.Polygraph of the not so distant future will look nothing like what we do today, I suspect. When and what that means is still a mystery, but it won't be too long until we're all antiques I'm sure. Researchers have all kinds of ideas, and some will likely take off, but what? We'll just have to wait and see. IP: Logged |
jrwygant Member
|
posted 06-25-2006 11:03 AM
Charles Honts sent me a bunch of stuff on DLC a few years ago and, if my memory has not failed me, there was a very precise process involved in administering a DLC test. It was not just a PLC test in which the examinee was told to lie. I recall that it included stimming the DLCs between every chart, something like "Now remember, you must tell a lie in your answer to that question about ___." I am uncomfortable with stimming any questions between charts, which might correspond to the "it doesn't feel right" argument. I am also uncomfortable with what amounts to an invitation to do countermeasures. In any event, I just sent Charles an email and asked him to take a look at these messages. I don't know if he's in town right now, but maybe he can contribute some firsthand knowledge.IP: Logged |
Barry C Member
|
posted 06-25-2006 11:33 AM
Jim,You're exactly right, but Charles is going to say he wrote exactly how to conduct such a test in Kleiner, which he to me I could feel free to distribute to anybody who wants it. Just email me, and I'll send it to whoever wants it. (The stimming between charts takes place on a Utah PLCQ test too. It's just done a little differently, but that's another topic.) It's not tough to run the tests (and the DoDPI guys do them a little differently, but they use them for screening exams as pointed out above), but you do have to understand the entire process, which to write here, would require re-writing Charles' and David Raskin's work. IP: Logged |
polypro Member
|
posted 06-25-2006 12:41 PM
I like the DLC as a backup to PLC's. I have them in my computer, and will not hesitate to put them to use if the need arises. However, I'm fully aware that one can create a countermeasure if the examiner isn't extremely careful. I think Paul referred to it as mental ping-pong. As I understand it, DLC's can be used in any CQT format, or am I completely out there somewhere in left field? This is a great dicussion, because, as I stated earlier, this may be something I can utlize in the future. I'm learning a great deal from everyone's input. ThanksIP: Logged |
Barry C Member
|
posted 06-26-2006 05:04 AM
You'll probably get some resistance if you use them in "any" format. Use them in any Utah test format, and you'll be fine. (I'd suggest using one of the published Utah versions; althouogh, Charles Honts will tell you that any CQT (PLCQ or DLCQ) with at least a couple well-developed RQs and a couple well-developed CQs is a valid CQT.)Score the test using the Utah scoring criteria as it has been validated on both DLCQ and PLCQ tests. Remember that the set-up of the test is crucial, so be sure your pre-test is correct, which, again, you'll find in the chapter in Kleiner. You need two articles to learn how to run Utah tests (DLCQ and PLCQ): the Honts/Raskin chapter in Kleiner, and the scoring article in the 1999 special edition fo Polygraph. IP: Logged |
Barry C Member
|
posted 06-26-2006 05:04 AM
Duplicate of above. How, I don't know.[This message has been edited by Barry C (edited 06-26-2006).] IP: Logged | |